Developing and testing a method of remotely improving younger students executive functions, volitional attention and auditory memory

Cover Page

Cite item

Full Text

Open Access Open Access
Restricted Access Access granted
Restricted Access Subscription Access

Abstract

During primary school age, children undergo a quick progression of voluntary actions that indicate cognitive predictors for academic triumph [1]. Modern research indicates that the effectiveness of educational activities heavily relies on the development of working memory, cognitive flexibility, and self-control, all of which are closely associated with brain control functions. Additionally, the level of auditory-speech memory development has a direct influence on reading skills development and the ability to articulate and convey thoughts [2, 3]. According to various sources, between 15 to 40% of schoolchildren in primary grades fall behind, which can cause significant long-term cognitive and social difficulties and lead to various psychosocial consequences. Thus, the importance of taking preventive measures for developing these functions in primary school has increased to prevent the formation of mental defects, which are major factors in a child‘s maladaptation in society.

The current study aims to analyze the effectiveness of the author‘s remote program “Kind Elephant” in improving the auditory-speech memory, voluntary attention, and control functions of younger elementary school students.

The program is designed to complete a range of tasks over a seven-week period, following standardized methods while also accounting for the neurodynamic parameters of children in this age group.

Seventy-six students between the ages of 7 and 9, in grades 1 and 2, were enrolled in the study. Of these, 44 completed the Kind Elephant development program, while the remaining 32 participants served as the control group.

With the assistance of the auditory-speech test and computer techniques, data was obtained on the development of control functions, voluntary attention, and auditory-speech memory in schoolchildren in two sections that were seven weeks apart. The techniques used were Analysis of Sentence Understanding, Bourdon‘s Proof-Reading Test, Hands-Legs-Head, and Understanding of Similar Sounding Words [4]. To investigate the assessment of children‘s programming and control functions, parents completed a BRIEF questionnaire (Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function) before and after the study [5]. An analysis was conducted to determine the significance of differences in indicators between the two groups‘ results.

This study demonstrates the potential for remote development techniques to enhance the ability to regulate behavior, maintain focus on specific activities, reduce fatigue, increase productivity, and improve auditory-speech memory volume.

Thus, younger schoolchildren‘s cognitive development can occur remotely, resolving access challenges for parents and enabling more children to keep pace with peers and thrive in social contexts. The authors maintain that using technology within reasonable limits can be acceptable for children‘s daily life if tailored to their mental maturation.

Full Text

During primary school age, children undergo a quick progression of voluntary actions that indicate cognitive predictors for academic triumph [1]. Modern research indicates that the effectiveness of educational activities heavily relies on the development of working memory, cognitive flexibility, and self-control, all of which are closely associated with brain control functions. Additionally, the level of auditory-speech memory development has a direct influence on reading skills development and the ability to articulate and convey thoughts [2, 3]. According to various sources, between 15 to 40% of schoolchildren in primary grades fall behind, which can cause significant long-term cognitive and social difficulties and lead to various psychosocial consequences. Thus, the importance of taking preventive measures for developing these functions in primary school has increased to prevent the formation of mental defects, which are major factors in a child‘s maladaptation in society.

The current study aims to analyze the effectiveness of the author‘s remote program “Kind Elephant” in improving the auditory-speech memory, voluntary attention, and control functions of younger elementary school students.

The program is designed to complete a range of tasks over a seven-week period, following standardized methods while also accounting for the neurodynamic parameters of children in this age group.

Seventy-six students between the ages of 7 and 9, in grades 1 and 2, were enrolled in the study. Of these, 44 completed the Kind Elephant development program, while the remaining 32 participants served as the control group.

With the assistance of the auditory-speech test and computer techniques, data was obtained on the development of control functions, voluntary attention, and auditory-speech memory in schoolchildren in two sections that were seven weeks apart. The techniques used were Analysis of Sentence Understanding, Bourdon‘s Proof-Reading Test, Hands-Legs-Head, and Understanding of Similar Sounding Words [4]. To investigate the assessment of children‘s programming and control functions, parents completed a BRIEF questionnaire (Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function) before and after the study [5]. An analysis was conducted to determine the significance of differences in indicators between the two groups‘ results.

This study demonstrates the potential for remote development techniques to enhance the ability to regulate behavior, maintain focus on specific activities, reduce fatigue, increase productivity, and improve auditory-speech memory volume.

Thus, younger schoolchildren‘s cognitive development can occur remotely, resolving access challenges for parents and enabling more children to keep pace with peers and thrive in social contexts. The authors maintain that using technology within reasonable limits can be acceptable for children‘s daily life if tailored to their mental maturation.

×

About the authors

T. R. Tomenko

Laboratory of Clinical and Behavioral Human Studies at the Ural Federal University named after the first President of Russia B.N. Yeltsin

Author for correspondence.
Email: m.a.trubnikova@gmail.com
Russian Federation, Yekaterinburg

M. A. Bogdanova

Laboratory of Clinical and Behavioral Human Studies at the Ural Federal University named after the first President of Russia B.N. Yeltsin

Email: m.a.trubnikova@gmail.com
Russian Federation, Yekaterinburg

References

  1. Leontev AN. Razvitie vysshikh form zapominaniya. Izbrannye psikhologicheskie trudy. Vol. 1. Moscow: Pedagogika; 1983. 392 p. (In Russ).
  2. Tikhomirova TN., Voronin IA, Misozhnikova EB, Malykh SB. Struktura vzaimosvyazei kognitivnykh kharakteristik i akademicheskoi uspeshnosti v shkolnom vozraste. Teoreticheskaya i eksperimentalnaya psikhologiya. 2015;8(2):55–68. (In Russ).
  3. Bauer JR, Booth AE. Exploring potential cognitive foundations of scientific literacy in preschoolers: Causal reasoning and executive function. Early Childhood Research Quarterly. 2019;46:275–284. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.09.007.
  4. Gorina EYu, Akhutina TV. Otsenka funktsii programmirovaniya i kontrolya u pervoklassnikov: neiropsikhologicheskoe obsledovanie, analiz oshibok na pisme, oprosnik BRIEF. Kulturno-istoricheskaya psikhologiya. 2011;3:105–113. (In Russ).
  5. Akhutina TV, Kremlev AE, Korneev AA, et al. Razrabotka kompyuternykh metodik neiropsikhologicheskogo obsledovaniya. Kognitivnaya nauka v Moskve: novye issledovaniya. Conference proceedigs June 15, 2017. Moscow: Buki Vedi; 2017:486–490.

Supplementary files

Supplementary Files
Action
1. JATS XML

Copyright (c) 2023 Eco-Vector

Creative Commons License
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.

Согласие на обработку персональных данных с помощью сервиса «Яндекс.Метрика»

1. Я (далее – «Пользователь» или «Субъект персональных данных»), осуществляя использование сайта https://journals.rcsi.science/ (далее – «Сайт»), подтверждая свою полную дееспособность даю согласие на обработку персональных данных с использованием средств автоматизации Оператору - федеральному государственному бюджетному учреждению «Российский центр научной информации» (РЦНИ), далее – «Оператор», расположенному по адресу: 119991, г. Москва, Ленинский просп., д.32А, со следующими условиями.

2. Категории обрабатываемых данных: файлы «cookies» (куки-файлы). Файлы «cookie» – это небольшой текстовый файл, который веб-сервер может хранить в браузере Пользователя. Данные файлы веб-сервер загружает на устройство Пользователя при посещении им Сайта. При каждом следующем посещении Пользователем Сайта «cookie» файлы отправляются на Сайт Оператора. Данные файлы позволяют Сайту распознавать устройство Пользователя. Содержимое такого файла может как относиться, так и не относиться к персональным данным, в зависимости от того, содержит ли такой файл персональные данные или содержит обезличенные технические данные.

3. Цель обработки персональных данных: анализ пользовательской активности с помощью сервиса «Яндекс.Метрика».

4. Категории субъектов персональных данных: все Пользователи Сайта, которые дали согласие на обработку файлов «cookie».

5. Способы обработки: сбор, запись, систематизация, накопление, хранение, уточнение (обновление, изменение), извлечение, использование, передача (доступ, предоставление), блокирование, удаление, уничтожение персональных данных.

6. Срок обработки и хранения: до получения от Субъекта персональных данных требования о прекращении обработки/отзыва согласия.

7. Способ отзыва: заявление об отзыве в письменном виде путём его направления на адрес электронной почты Оператора: info@rcsi.science или путем письменного обращения по юридическому адресу: 119991, г. Москва, Ленинский просп., д.32А

8. Субъект персональных данных вправе запретить своему оборудованию прием этих данных или ограничить прием этих данных. При отказе от получения таких данных или при ограничении приема данных некоторые функции Сайта могут работать некорректно. Субъект персональных данных обязуется сам настроить свое оборудование таким способом, чтобы оно обеспечивало адекватный его желаниям режим работы и уровень защиты данных файлов «cookie», Оператор не предоставляет технологических и правовых консультаций на темы подобного характера.

9. Порядок уничтожения персональных данных при достижении цели их обработки или при наступлении иных законных оснований определяется Оператором в соответствии с законодательством Российской Федерации.

10. Я согласен/согласна квалифицировать в качестве своей простой электронной подписи под настоящим Согласием и под Политикой обработки персональных данных выполнение мною следующего действия на сайте: https://journals.rcsi.science/ нажатие мною на интерфейсе с текстом: «Сайт использует сервис «Яндекс.Метрика» (который использует файлы «cookie») на элемент с текстом «Принять и продолжить».