Specificity of experience psychological time and dysfunctional metacognitive models as consequences of experience traumatic events in childhood
- Authors: Andronnikova O.O.1, Romanova N.V.1
-
Affiliations:
- Novosibirsk State Pedagogical University
- Issue: Vol 16, No 2 (2025)
- Pages: 555-577
- Section: Psychological Studies
- Published: 30.04.2025
- URL: https://journal-vniispk.ru/2658-4034/article/view/302895
- DOI: https://doi.org/10.12731/2658-4034-2025-16-2-709
- EDN: https://elibrary.ru/JEKDFV
- ID: 302895
Cite item
Full Text
Abstract
Background. The traumatic experience, which is quite common, is not perceived linearly. The memory of trauma is often subjectively felt outside the chronological structure of ordinary life, “stuck” and repeating itself as a fragment, disrupting the sense of reality and its temporal processes. The resulting temporal distortion or gap hinders the process of overcoming traumatic experience. Dysfunctional metacognitive models and ineffective coping strategies that appear against the background of impaired temporal perception contribute to the development of the consequences of traumatization and hinder post-traumatic growth. The relevance of studying the temporal matrix of experiencing traumatic experience lies in its significance for a comprehensive understanding of how time is transformed and experienced under the influence of psychological trauma.
Purpose – analysis of the structural components and functioning of the temporal matrix of traumatic experience, revealing the patterns of determination by traumatic events experienced in childhood, the specifics of psychological time and dysfunctional metacognitions that disrupt personality adaptation.
Materials and methods. The study was conducted on a sample of 101 (78 women and 23 men) adults aged 18 to 54 years (M = 31.80, SD = 8.32). The study included a survey of participants using questionnaires and testing procedures. The following methods were used: a questionnaire to study the demographic and biographical data of the subjects; a questionnaire aimed at studying the degree of victimization (Victimization protocol); F. Zimbardo's time perspective questionnaire; a questionnaire of metacognitive beliefs; a questionnaire of coping with stress COPE-30. To study the main relationships between the variables, identify the factor structure and multiple regression, the SPSS version 22.0 program was used.
Results. There is a relationship between traumatic experience, dysfunctional metacognitions, time perspectives, strategies of avoiding experience and denial. A factor structure was identified, which we called the "temporal matrix of traumatic experience" (or "traumatic temporal matrix"), including five components: victimization in the past and present, violation of psychological time, dysfunctional coping strategies, dysfunctional metacognitions, and avoidance of experience. The results of regression for the dependent variable showed the determination of the factor structure by the presence of traumatic experience. Thus, experiencing traumatic events leads to the emergence of a specific rigid construct consisting of a negative assessment of the past and fixation on it, with a fatalistic perception of the present; dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs, including distrust of one's own perceptual processes and a belief in the inability to control emotions; dysfunctional coping strategies and strategies for avoiding painful experience. This study increases the awareness of specialists in the specifics of experiencing traumatic experience and can be used in practice aimed at post-traumatic growth.
About the authors
Ol’ga O. Andronnikova
Novosibirsk State Pedagogical University
Author for correspondence.
Email: andronnikova_69@mail.ru
ORCID iD: 0000-0002-1756-7682
SPIN-code: 9118-8231
Scopus Author ID: 57193952779
ResearcherId: D-2926-2014
PhD in Phycology, Associate Professor, Dean of Faculty of Phycology
Russian Federation, 28, Vilyuiskaya Str., Novosibirsk, 630126, Russian FederationNatalia V. Romanova
Novosibirsk State Pedagogical University
Email: vita80@inbox.ru
ORCID iD: 0009-0005-6345-6376
SPIN-code: 1939-4383
Applicant, Psychotherapeutic Center "Berkana"
Russian Federation, 28, Vilyuiskaya Str., Novosibirsk, 630126, Russian FederationReferences
- Li, T. (2024). Psychological Well-being of Personality in the Context of Time as a Psychological Phenomenon. Herzen Readings: Psychological Research in Education, (7), 539–547. https://doi.org/10.33910/herzenpsyconf-2024-7-75 EDN: https://elibrary.ru/IJAQHB
- Odintsova, M. A., Radchikova, N. P., & Aleksandrova, L. A. (2022). SORE-30: Psychometric Properties of a Short Version of the Russian Coping Strategies Questionnaire. Bulletin of Moscow University. Series 14. Psychology, (4), 247–275. https://doi.org/10.11621/vsp.2022.04.11 EDN: https://elibrary.ru/VSPZXI
- Siorta, N. A., Moskvitchenko, D. V., Yaltovskiy, V. M., & Yaltonskaya, A. V. (2018). Validation of a Short Version of the Metacognitive Beliefs Questionnaire on a Russian Sample. Psikhologiya. ZHurnal Vysshey shkoly ekonomiki, (2). https://doi.org/10.17323/1813-8918-2018-2-307-325 EDN: https://elibrary.ru/ZAHPPV
- Syrtseva, A., Sokolova, E., & Mitina, O. (2008). Adaptation of the Temporal Perspective Scale by Philip Zimbardo. Psychological Journal, (3), 101–109.
- Bolotova, A. K., & Hachaturova, M. R. (2013). The Role of Time Perspective in Coping Behavior. Psychology in Russia: State of the Art, 6(3), 120–131. https://doi.org/10.11621/pir.2013.0311 EDN: https://elibrary.ru/RZSVSP
- Bowlby, J., Ainsworth, M., & Bretherton, I. (1992). The Origins of Attachment Theory. Developmental Psychology, 28, 759–775.
- Brinck, I., & Liljenfors, R. (2013). The Developmental Origin of Metacognition. Infant and Child Development, 22(1), 85–101.
- Gökkaya, F., Yurdalan, F., & Çıvgın, U. (2025). Does Time Perspective Affect the Association Between Childhood Traumas and Post-traumatic Growth? Curr Psychol. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-025-07300-5
- Hosseini Ramaghani, N. A., Rezaei, F., Sepahvandi, M. A., Gholamrezaei, S., & Mirderikvand, F. (2019). The Mediating Role of Metacognition, Time Perspectives and Experiential Avoidance on the Relationship Between Childhood Trauma and Post-traumatic Stress Disorder Symptoms. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 10(1), 1648173. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2019.1648173
- Karan, A. (2021). The Relationship Between Childhood Trauma and Dissociative Experiences: The Mediator Role of Time Perspective and Emotion Regulation. Master Thesis, İşık Üniversitesi, Ankara. 111 p.
- Kline, N. K., Berke, D. S., Rhodes, C. A., Steenkamp, M. M., & Litz, B. T. (2018). Self-blame and PTSD Following Sexual Assault: A Longitudinal Analysis. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 36(5-6), 3153–3168. https://doi.org/10.1177/0886260518770652
- Myers, S. G., & Wells, A. (2015). Early Trauma, Negative Affect, and Anxious Attachment: The Role of Metacognition. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 28(6), 634–649. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2015.1009832
- Schneider, W. (2008). The Development of Metacognitive Knowledge in Children and Adolescents: Major Trends and Implications for Education. Mind, Brain, and Education, 2(3), 114–121. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-228X.2008.00041.x
- Stojić, S., & Nadasdy, Z. (2024). Event as the Central Construal of Psychological Time in Humans. Front. Psychol., 15, 1402903. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1402903
- Stolarski, M., & Cyniak-Cieciura, M. (2016). Balanced and Less Traumatized: Balanced Time Perspective Mediates the Relationship Between Temperament and Severity of PTSD Syndrome in Motor Vehicle Accident Survivor Sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 101, 456–461. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.055 EDN: https://elibrary.ru/WOTUSL
- Treitel, C., & Levy-Gigi, E. (2024). The Dual Role of Time Perception in Trauma-Exposed Individuals: A Conceptual Review. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 16(8), 1392–1400. https://doi.org/10.1037/tra0001631 EDN: https://elibrary.ru/HHYNNO
- van Wassenhove, V. (2023). Cognitive Neuroscience of Time: Nows, Timelines, and Chronologies. Time and Science, 2, 97–129. https://doi.org/10.1142/9781800613751_0005
- Vang, M. L., Shevlin, M., Karatzias, T., Fyvie, C., & Hyland, P. (2018). Dissociation Fully Mediates the Relationship Between Childhood Sexual and Emotional Abuse and DSM-5 PTSD in a Sample of Treatment-Seeking Adults. European Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 2(4), 173–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ejtd.2018.02.004
- Zambianchi, M. (2018). Time Perspective, Coping Styles, Perceived Efficacy in Affect Regulation, and Creative Problem Solving in Adolescence and Youth. Psicología Educativa, 24, 1–6. https://doi.org/10.5093/psed2018a1 EDN: https://elibrary.ru/VEAWQT
- Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (2008). The Time Paradox: The New Psychology of Time That Will Change Your Life. Simon and Schuster. 339 p.
- Zimbardo, P. G., Sword, R., & Sword, R. (2012). The Time Cure. San Francisco, CA: Wiley. 336 p.
Supplementary files
